How to Appraise and Use an Article about Therapy

Chapter 3. How to Appraise and Use an Article about Therapy

Romina Brignardello-Petersen, D.D.S., M.Sc., Ph.D.; Alonso Carrasco-Labra, D.D.S., M.Sc., Ph.D.; Michael Glick, D.M.D.; Gordon H. Guyatt, M.D., M.Sc.; and Amir Azarpazhooh, D.D.S., M.Sc., Ph.D.

Introduction

In the two previous chapters in this book, we introduced the process of evidence-based dentistry1 and explained how to search for evidence to inform clinical practice.2 In this chapter, we explain how to use a research report to inform clinical decisions pertaining to questions of therapy. We will introduce and describe the basic concepts for understanding randomized controlled trials (RCTs), and we explain how to critically appraise such studies. In subsequent chapters in this book, we describe how to use other types of study designs.

Clinical Questions of Therapy

Dental practitioners spend most of their time administering treatments to their patients. A therapy or treatment can be defined as “any intervention, which may include prescribing drugs, performing surgery, or counseling, that is intended to improve the course of disease once it is established.”3 Many of the clinical questions that arise in clinical practice have to do with the effectiveness of treatments or interventions.

Box 3.1. Clinical Scenario

You referred a patient to an oral and maxillofacial surgeon for surgical extraction of an impacted mandibular third molar that has been causing the patient to have repeated episodes of pericoronitis. Your patient returned from the surgery consultation session and told you that the surgeon explained the risk of a postoperative infection and how to avoid it. The surgeon gave your patient the option to use either chlorhexidine gel or chlorhexidine mouth rinse. Your patient now is asking for your opinion regarding which of these two options may be more effective. You are not sure, so you decide to search for evidence from a clinical study to answer this question.

As described by the authors of Chapter 2 of this book, therapy questions can be stated using the population, intervention, comparison, outcomes (PICO) framework.4 The population is the patients who are to receive the intervention, the intervention is the treatment of interest, the comparison is the reference to which we are comparing the intervention, and the outcomes are the health consequences that depend on the intervention. The comparison can be a different treatment or no treatment at all. Table 3.1 shows two examples of therapy questions and their corresponding questions in the PICO framework: one of them has no treatment as the comparison and the other has an alternative treatment.

Table 3.1. Examples of Therapy Questions and the PICO* Framework

image

* PICO = population, intervention, comparison, outcomes.

What Study Design Best Addresses Questions of Therapy?

At the level of primary studies, RCTs represent the optimal study design to address questions of therapy. An RCT is an experiment assessing a medical treatment in patients.5 In an RCT, participants are allocated randomly into two or more groups that are treated equally except for the intervention the participants receive. After the intervention is applied, investigators follow patients over a specified time and measure outcomes, ideally those that are important to patients. If the study has been well designed and implemented, we can attribute differences that arise to the treatment under investigation (Figure 3.1).3

Figure 3.1. Randomized Clinical Trial

image

One RCT in dentistry, conducted by Hita-Iglesias and colleagues,6 addressed whether chlorhexidine gel or chlorhexidine rinse was more effective in preventing alveolar osteitis following third-molar extraction. After the surgery was performed, the investigators randomly allocated patients to receive either the gel or the rinse for one week, and evaluated patients on the third and seventh days postextraction to determine the presence of alveolar osteitis.

RCTs are the best type of study design to determine the effectiveness of an intervention because they minimize bias—a systematic deviation from the underlying truth7—by ensuring that patients in the intervention and control groups are similar with respect to factors that determine whether the outcome of interest will occur. If well designed, RCTs also control events that occur after randomization and most aspects of the course of events, such as how the outcomes are measured.8 Therefore, clinicians should aim to inform their clinical decisions regarding therapy using individual RCTs or, even better, systematic reviews of RCTs. In this chapter, we will focus on how to use stand-alone RCTs, and in subsequent chapters in this book, we will describe how to use systematic reviews.

Box 3.2. The Study You Found

During your search, you did not identify any summary or systematic review; however, you did find a randomized controlled trial (RCT) that seems to answer your question. The investigators of this RCT addressed whether postoperative chlorhexidine gel or chlorhexidine rinse was effective for preventing alveolar osteitis after extracting mandibular third molars.6 You read the abstract of this RCT, which indicated that the researchers recruited 73 patients and followed them for one week after the third-molar extraction, and it seemed as though chlorhexidine gel was more effective than the mouth rinse. However, you decide that you need to read the entire article to review more details before accepting its results and deciding whether the results are applicable to your patient.

Critically Appraising an RCT to Inform Clinical Decisions

The process of using an article from the dental literature to inform clinical decisions involves assessing the risk of bias, the results, and the applicability of the results.9 Below, we describe each of these three steps.

How Serious Is the Risk of Bias?

The extent to which a study’s results are likely to be correct for the sample of patients enrolled10 depends on how well the study was designed and conducted. Investigators of RCTs strive to ensure that determinants of the outcome of interest (factors such as age, sex, and disease severity, which we call prognostic factors) other than the treatment under investigation are similar between the groups being compared at the start of the study, and that these determinants remain similar throughout the study.8 Only if investigators achieve and maintain prognostic balance can we be sure that any differences in outcomes are owing to the intervention (and not to bias introduced by the prognostic factors). Table 3.21116 presents the aspects to consider when assessing the risk of bias of an RCT addressing a question of therapy.

Table 3.2. Critically Appraising the Risk of Bias in an Article About Therapy*

Aspect

Example

Explanation

1. Did intervention and control groups start with the same prognosis?

1a. Were patients randomized?

“The sites presenting class II furcation lesions were randomly assigned, by a computer-generated list, to receive PDT [photodynamic therapy] or non-activated laser/only photo- sensitizer, both following [scaling and root planing].”11

Authors described that the sites were allocated randomly to the treatment groups and how the randomization sequence was generated. Examples of appropriate methods for generating the randomization sequence are random number tables, computer generators, and coin tossing. Inappropriate methods are those that do not produce true randomization, such as assigning patients to the groups on the basis of their date of birth or admission, or according to their record numbers.12

1b. Was randomization concealed?

“For each center, 16 consecutively numbered, opaque, sealed envelopes containing a note with the treatment (eight for each treatment) were made and placed in a larger envelope. For each patient, an independent person at each center randomly drew an envelope and handed it to Dentist B. This was repeated until 16 patients at each center were included.”13

Authors described using consecutively numbered, opaque, sealed envelopes, which were handed to the clinician by an independent person. This is an appropriate method for concealing the allocation. Other adequate methods include the use of sequentially numbered drug containers, and—by far the best of all—central allocation (telephone, web based, or pharmacy controlled). Allocation schedules or lists, envelopes without safeguards, or alternation are not appropriate for concealing allocation.12

1c. Were patients in the study groups similar with respect to known prognostic factors?

“There were no significant differences between groups for age, gender, duration of DM, glycemic status and category of DM regimen.”14

The authors presented a table with the baseline characteristics (potential prognostic factors) of the patients per group. When assessing this aspect, it is important to determine whether all relevant prognostic factors were considered.

2. Was prognostic balance maintained as the study progressed?

2a. To what extent was the study blinded?

“The entire study was blinded. The prophylaxis paste cups used had silver/blank lidstock and were only identified by a letter on the lidstock. The groups were not known by the examiners or patients. The examiner was in a different section of the building and the study coordinator gave the paste to the hygienist in yet another location of the building.”15

Authors described that their study was blinded. They also mentioned that the patients, the person administering the treatment (hygienist), and the examiners (outcome assessors) were blinded, and they described how this was achieved. Ideally, the authors should have mentioned that the data analyst was blinded as well. If blinding was not possible, it is necessary to judge the extent that this could have influenced the outcome measurement.12

3. Were the groups prognostically balanced at the study’s completion?

3a. Was follow-up complete?

“Analyses of the dropouts revealed no differences in TMJ pain, physical functioning, emotional functioning, or demographic data compared to the patients who completed the study.” The information provided in the study flowchart shows that the number of patients lost to follow-up was one in one group (patients refused participation) and three in the other (one patient refused participation and two moved from the area) at 10 weeks after the intervention.13

Authors described the number and reasons for losing patients to follow-up. They also assessed whether these patients were different from those who continued in the study and found no differences. This showed that the risk of bias owing to incomplete follow-up was low. Other methods to assess this are to check whether the number of patients lost to follow-up and the reasons are similar between groups, whether the proportion of patients lost to follow-up is high enough to change the results if they were not missing, or to perform data imputation and draw conclusions on the basis of the results.12

3b. Were patients analyzed in the groups to which they were randomized?

“Trial outcomes were analysed by intention to treat. Per-treatment and per-protocol analyses of trial outcomes were also done for comparison.”16

Authors mentioned that they performed an intention-to-treat analysis. They described that they also did a per-protocol analysis to compare the results of both.

3c. Was the trial stopped early?

“[I]t was determined that 16 subjects per group would be necessary to provide an 80% power with an alpha of 0.05. . . . Thirty-eight subjects. . . . were selected from the population referred to the Periodontal Clinic of Guarulhos University.”14

Authors described the calculation of the target sample size, and later they mentioned the number of patients recruited, from which it can be inferred that they did not stop the trial early. The authors of the trials that have been stopped early owing to some data-dependent process usually mention this.

* Sources: Luchesi and colleagues,11 Higgins and colleagues,12 Christidis and colleagues,13 Santos and colleagues,14 Neuhaus and colleagues,15 and Kelleher and colleagues.16

DM = diabetes mellitus.

TMJ = temporomandibular joint.

image Did the intervention and control groups start with the same prognosis?

One key aspect of a study that can help answer a question regarding therapy is whether the groups were balanced with regard to prognostic factors at the beginning of the study. Investigators can achieve this balance through their control of how the patients are allocated to the intervention and control groups.

Randomization assigns patients to the intervention or control groups by chance.8 The goal is to ensure that both known and unknown prognostic factors are distributed similarly in the intervention and control groups and, thus, avoid bias.9, 17 This is why, when therapy questions are to be answered, the use of RCTs is superior to the use of observational studies: when the decision of what treatment to provide to a given patient is left to the dentist (as is done in observational studies), it is likely that patient characteristics may influence the choice of therapy. For instance, because of its ease of use, dentists may prefer to use chlorhexidine mouth rinse rather than gel in older patients who have a greater risk of experiencing subsequent infection, and to use the chlorhexidine gel in younger patients who have a lower risk of experiencing subsequent infection.

Even a well-prepared (typically, computer-generated) randomization schedule does not ensure random allocation. If those enrolling patients are aware of the treatment (intervention or control) to which the next patient will be allocated, they may make choices that undermine randomization.18 For example, if the next scheduled patient is an older person, and a member of the research staff who is responsible for allocation believes that the assigned treatment of gel is not optimal for the patient, the staff member may manipulate the allocation to ensure that the patient receives the mouth rinse.

To prevent this manipulation of the randomization schedule, those recruiting patients should not be informed about which group the next patient will be allocated to. This strategy is called “allocation concealment.”19 The authors of one study found that the investigators of trials in which allocation concealment was either inadequate or unclear reported treatment effects approximately 40% larger than those reported by studies with adequate allocation concealment.20

Particularly if the sample size is small, even concealed randomization may fail to do its job of ensuring prognostic balance. It is important, therefore, to check whether the baseline characteristics of the patients in both groups are similar.9

In summary, concealed random allocation with evidence that patients in the intervention and control groups started with the same prognosis reassures us that we are likely to obtain unbiased estimates of treatment effect.

image Was prognostic balance maintained as the study progressed?

Awareness on the part of dentists administering the intervention, patients who receive the care, or those measuring the outcomes in the study of whether patients are receiving intervention or control treatment can influence their behavior. For instance, the judgment of whether osteitis occurs involves some subjectivity, and it is possible that an outcome assessor who favors using gel over wash will be more likely to diagnose osteitis in those receiving the wash.21 Blinding (also known as masking) refers to investigators ensuring that patients, clinicians, and those collecting data and adjudicating whether an outcome has occurred are unaware of whether patients are receiving the intervention or the control.9

Typically, trials are described as double-blinded, which may mean that none of the patients, clinicians, data collectors, or outcome assessors are aware of what treatment the patient was receiving.21 The term “double-blind” does not leave us confident that all four groups are unaware of allocation, and optimal reporting will inform us whether this is the case.9

The method to achieve blinding in RCTs is the use of a placebo. A placebo is a treatment that resembles the intervention of interest but has no biological effect.8 Most placebos are pills and are administered in the same way as the real drug. Many of the treatments administered in dentistry, however, are procedures. In this case, although doing so is ethically arguable, investigators of the most rigorously designed trials could use a sham procedure to blind patients.22

Many times, however, it is not possible to blind patients or clinicians. Fortunately, lack of blinding will not always result in bias. For instance, it may not be possible to blind the clinician because two materials with different appearance and techniques of use are being compared. If, however, patients and outcome assessors are blinded, and there are no other treatments that affect the outcome that the dentists may administer differentially to intervention and control patients (known as cointervention), not blinding the dentist may not affect the results. Therefore, when evaluating whether prognostic balance was maintained as the study progresses, not only do we have to assess whether the groups of interest were blinded, but also we must consider whether any lack of blinding may have caused any bias.21

image Were the groups prognostically balanced at the completion of the study?

Strategies to maintain the prognostic balance as the study proceeds include following up on all patients, analyzing them in the group to which they were allocated, and completing the trial as planned.9

There are circumstances in which researchers are not able to follow up and measure the outcome for some patients (referred to as “lost to follow-up”). Patients lost to follow-up may well have different prognoses from those who remain until the end of the study,9 and hence, the similarity of prognosis in intervention and control groups may be compromised. Ideally researchers should know the outcomes of all participants in a trial (or at least know the reasons why some patients were lost to follow-up).

If the proportion of patients lost to follow-up is so small that including their results in the data analysis would not change the overall results even if all of them had the best or worst outcome, and if the reasons for losing those patients are reported in the trial and are not potentially related to the outcome (for example, a patient moved to another city and could not attend the follow-up visits), then the risk of bias does not increase materially.

Sometimes patients do not receive the intervention as intended because they do not adhere to instructions. Patients may even receive the treatment meant for the patients allocated to the other group. If nonadherence to a treatment is related to a prognostic factor, excluding these patients from the analysis and using data only from patients who adhered to the treatment (that is, doing a per-protocol analysis) will destroy the prognostic balance achieved and maintained through all the previous strategies. To avoid this, researchers use the “intention-to-treat” principle; that is, they analyze the data from each patient in the group to which the patient was allocated. In this way, they get an unbiased estimate of the effect of the intervention at the level of adherence observed in the trial.23

Randomization only can ensure prognostic balance when the sample size is large; when the sample size is small, chance can result in large prognostic imbalance. As a result, early in an RCT when sample sizes are small, results may not be indicators of the future overall results even if the RCT otherwise is designed meticulously.24 Therefore, it can be misleading if investigators stop a trial early on the basis that they have seen a large effect.9 Both simulations and empirical data show that, on average, the results of trials that were stopped early because of a perceived benefit overestimate treatment effects, and the smaller the number of patients randomized as well as the number of outcome events observed at the time the trial is stopped, the larger the overestimate of effect.25 Thus, a final criterion to determine whether the intervention and control groups were balanced prognostically at the end of the study is to assess whether the trial was stopped early.

In summary, prognostic balance is maintained up to the completion of the study if there are few participants who are lost to follow-up, if the authors performed an intention-to-treat analysis, and if the trial was completed as planned.

Box 3.3. Your Assessment of the Risk of Bias of the Randomized Controlled Trial You Identified

With respect to initial prognostic balance, you find that patients were randomized adequately and that the randomization schedule was likely to be concealed in an appropriate way. Nevertheless, there were some differences between groups in the distribution of men and women and in the distribution of smokers. With respect to maintaining prognostic balance as the study progressed, only the surgeons were blinded to the interventions the participants received. With respect to prognostic balance at the study completion, the authors did not describe how many, if any, patients were lost to follow-up in each treatment group. Your judgment leads you to consider that the lack of blinding of some participants and the lack of information regarding the losses to follow-up may have biased the results, so you decide to keep reading this study with caution (see the Supplemental Table6 at the end of this chapter for more details).

Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free dental videos. Join our Telegram channel

Aug 4, 2021 | Posted by in General Dentistry | Comments Off on How to Appraise and Use an Article about Therapy

VIDEdental - Online dental courses

Get VIDEdental app for watching clinical videos